Many Democrats are thrilled that former Rep. Beto O'Rourke of Texas has tossed his hat into the presidential ring. Ever since his near-upset of Sen. Ted Cruz last November, Beto (with whom we all seem to be on a first-name basis) has been a media darling — and scarcely a profile can be written about him without mentioning his charisma.
"Charisma has propelled him far," says Vox. "Beto's been compared to Obama and been confused for a Kennedy." A Forbes story details his "seven steps to charisma." He's "young and charismatic," says Politico.
You get the idea.
It's easy to understand the longing for a charismatic candidate. Crowds turn out, voters get excited — young voters especially — and the next thing anyone knows, the election's a done deal, with turnout effects all the way down the ticket.
But is charisma a good thing? Not according to Jessica Flanigan, a philosopher at the Jepson School at the University of Richmond. In a thoughtful 2013 paper titled "Charisma and Moral Reasoning," she argues that the quality that is so regularly praised "is intrinsically problematic from a moral perspective."
How can this be, when people get so excited about charismatic leadership? For Flanigan, the excitement itself turns out to be a problem. When people get excited about a leader, they tend to defer to that leader's decisions — in effect, to indulge a presumption that the leader will find the right answer. They do less reasoning on their own. "The problem with charisma," she writes, "is either that it inspires followers to act rightly but for the wrong reasons, or worse, that it inspires people to act wrongly for the wrong reasons."
Flanigan supports her arguments with extensive attention to social science. To take just one example, she cites a troubling 2009 study in which subjects were randomly assigned to different groups that were then asked to work cooperatively to solve mathematics problems. The researchers found that the groups did not select as their leaders the members with the greatest mathematical ability; instead they selected for confidence and dominance of personality.
In Flanigan's view, it's better to follow, say, the Rev. Martin Luther King Jr., because his argument is persuasive than because he's a "commanding and impressive" speaker. After all, even a commanding and impressive speaker can be wrong. Thus, she concludes, we should resist the appeal of charismatic leadership and search for other qualities instead.